forked from eggplantbren/STATS331
-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 0
/
Copy pathuncertainty.tex
108 lines (98 loc) · 5.97 KB
/
uncertainty.tex
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
\chapter{Introduction}
Every day, throughout our lives, we are required
to believe certain things and not to believe other things. This applies not
only to the ``big questions'' of life, but also to trivial matters, and
everything in between. For example, this morning I boarded the bus to
university, sure that it would actually take me here and not to Wellington.
How did I know the bus would not take me to Wellington? Well, for starters
I have taken the same bus many times before and it has always taken me to the
university. Another clue was that the bus said ``Midtown'' on it, and a bus
to Wellington probably would have said Wellington, and would not have stopped
at a minor bus stop in suburban Auckland.
None of this evidence {\it proves} that the bus would take me to university,
but it does makes it very {\it plausible}. Given all these pieces of information, I feel
quite certain that the bus will take me to the city. I feel so certain
about this that the possibility of an
unplanned trip to Wellington never even entered my mind until I decided to
write this paragraph.\\
Somehow, our brains are very often able to accurately predict the correct answer
to many questions (e.g. the destination of a bus), even though we don't have
all the available information that we would need to be 100\% certain.
We do this using our experience of the world and our intuition, usually
without much conscious attention or problem solving. However, there are areas
of study where we can't just use our intuition to make judgments like this.
For example, most of science involves such situations. Does a new treatment
work better than an old one? Is the expansion of the universe really
accelerating? People tend to be
interested in trying to answer questions that haven't been answered yet, so our
attention is always on the questions where we're not sure of the answer.
This is where statistics comes in as a tool to help us in this grey area, when we can't
be 100\% certain about things, but we still want to do the best we can with our
incomplete information.\\
\section{Certainty, Uncertainty and Probability}
In the above example, I said things like ``I couldn't be 100\% certain''. The
idea of using a number to describe how certain you are is quite natural.
For example, contestants on the TV show ``Who Wants to be a Millionaire'' often say things
like ``I'm 75\% sure the answer is A''\footnote{This reminds me of an amusing
exchange from the TV show {\it Monk}. {\bf Captain Stottlemeyer}: [about someone electrocuting her husband] Monk, are you sure? I mean, are you really sure? And don't give me any of that ``95 percent'' crap.
{\bf Monk}: Captain, I am 100\% sure... that she {\it probably} killed him.}.\\
There are some interesting
things to notice about this statement. Firstly, it is a subjective statement.
If someone else were in the seat trying to answer the question, she might say
the probability that A is correct is 100\%, because she knows the answer!
A third person faced with the same question might say the probability is 25\%,
because he has no idea and only knows that one of the four answers
must be correct.\\
In Bayesian statistics, the interpretation of what {\it probability} means is
that it is a description of {\it how certain you are that some statement, or
proposition, is true}.
If the probability is 1, you are sure that the statement is true. So sure, in
fact, that nothing could ever change your mind (we will demonstrate this in class).
If the probability is 0, you
are sure that the proposition is false. If the probability is 0.5, then you
are as uncertain as you would be about a fair coin flip. If the probability is
0.95, then you're quite sure the statement is true, but it wouldn't be {\it too}
surprising to you if you found out the statement was false. See
Figure~\ref{fig:probability_scale} for a graphical depiction of probabilities
as degrees of certainty or plausibility.\\
\begin{figure}[!ht]
\begin{center}
\includegraphics[scale=0.6]{Figures/probability_scale.pdf}
\caption{\it Probability can be used to describe degrees of certainty, or
how plausible some statement is. 0 and 1 are the two extremes of the scale
and correspond to complete certainty. However, probabilities are not static
quantities. When you get more information, your probabilities can change.
\label{fig:probability_scale}}
\end{center}
\end{figure}
\begin{center}
\begin{tabular}{|c|}
\hline
{\bf In Bayesian statistics, probabilities are in the mind, not in the world.}\\
\hline
\end{tabular}
\end{center}
It might sound like there is nothing more to Bayesian statistics than just
thinking about a question and then blurting out a probability that feels
appropriate. Fortunately for us, there's more to it than that! To see why, think
about how you change your mind when new evidence (such as a data set) becomes
available. For example, you may be on ``Who Wants to be a Millionaire?'' and
not know the answer to a question, so you might think the probability that it is
$A$ is 25\%. But if you call your friend using ``phone a friend'', and
your friend says,
``It's definitely $A$'', then you would be much more confident that it is $A$!
Your probability probably wouldn't go all the way to 100\% though, because there
is always the small possibility that your friend is mistaken.\\
\begin{framed}
{\bf When we get new information, we should {\it update} our probabilities to
take the new information into account. Bayesian methods tell us exactly how to
do this.}
\end{framed}
In this course, we will learn how to do {\it data analysis} from a Bayesian
point of view. So while the discussion in this chapter might sound a bit
like philosophy, we will see that using this kind of thinking can give us
new and powerful ways of solving practical data analysis problems. The methods we will use
will all have a common structure, so if you are faced with a completely new
data analysis problem one day, you will be able to design your own analysis
methods by using the Bayesian framework. Best of all, the methods make sense
and perform extremely well in practice!